Advertisement
Review| Volume 84, ISSUE 9, P634-643, November 01, 2018

Download started.

Ok

The Predictive Coding Account of Psychosis

Open AccessPublished:May 25, 2018DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.05.015

      Abstract

      Fueled by developments in computational neuroscience, there has been increasing interest in the underlying neurocomputational mechanisms of psychosis. One successful approach involves predictive coding and Bayesian inference. Here, inferences regarding the current state of the world are made by combining prior beliefs with incoming sensory signals. Mismatches between prior beliefs and incoming signals constitute prediction errors that drive new learning. Psychosis has been suggested to result from a decreased precision in the encoding of prior beliefs relative to the sensory data, thereby garnering maladaptive inferences. Here, we review the current evidence for aberrant predictive coding and discuss challenges for this canonical predictive coding account of psychosis. For example, hallucinations and delusions may relate to distinct alterations in predictive coding, despite their common co-occurrence. More broadly, some studies implicate weakened prior beliefs in psychosis, and others find stronger priors. These challenges might be answered with a more nuanced view of predictive coding. Different priors may be specified for different sensory modalities and their integration, and deficits in each modality need not be uniform. Furthermore, hierarchical organization may be critical. Altered processes at lower levels of a hierarchy need not be linearly related to processes at higher levels (and vice versa). Finally, canonical theories do not highlight active inference—the process through which the effects of our actions on our sensations are anticipated and minimized. It is possible that conflicting findings might be reconciled by considering these complexities, portending a framework for psychosis more equipped to deal with its many manifestations.

      Keywords

      There is a pressing need to understand and better treat psychosis (i.e., psychotic symptoms and psychotic disorders). While dopamine antagonists are effective, many patients experience residual symptoms (
      • Schennach R.
      • Riedel M.
      • Obermeier M.
      • Spellmann I.
      • Musil R.
      • Jager M.
      • et al.
      What are residual symptoms in schizophrenia spectrum disorder? Clinical description and 1-year persistence within a naturalistic trial.
      ). They have poor functional outcome and a high risk of suicide (
      • Togay B.
      • Noyan H.
      • Tasdelen R.
      • Ucok A.
      Clinical variables associated with suicide attempts in schizophrenia before and after the first episode.
      ). Furthermore, the side effects of many antipsychotics can lead to poor adherence. Here, we argue that single-level accounts of psychosis, such as the dopamine hypothesis, are too reductionist on their own and will achieve full value only when embedded in a more complex explanatory framework that unites several levels of explanation [e.g., Maia and Frank (
      • Maia T.V.
      • Frank M.J.
      An integrative perspective on the role of dopamine in schizophrenia.
      )]. Predictive coding and Bayesian inference (
      • Rao R.P.
      • Ballard D.H.
      Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects.
      ,
      • Friston K.J.
      A theory of cortical responses.
      ,
      • Clark A.
      Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science.
      ) may provide such a framework, linking the neurobiology of psychosis with its clinical phenomenology by way of computational processes. We will critically evaluate this framework and suggest future lines of inquiry.

      Predictive Coding as Hierarchical Bayesian Inference

      Von Helmholtz’s (
      • von Helmholtz H.
      Handbuch der physiologischen Optik.
      ) idea of unconscious inference held that the brain uses learned predictions to infer the causes of incoming sensory data. This process can be formalized as Bayesian inference (
      • Friston K.J.
      A theory of cortical responses.
      ,
      • Lee T.S.
      • Mumford D.
      Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex.
      ), whereby a probabilistic prediction (prior) is combined with observed sensory data (likelihood) to compute a posterior probability (posterior). The posterior corresponds to the percept that is most likely, given the prior and the likelihood (
      • Hohwy J.
      Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain.
      ). This may be implemented in the brain through predictive coding, but there are alternatives (
      • Jardri R.
      • Deneve S.
      Circular inferences in schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Phillips W.A.
      • Clark A.
      • Silverstein S.M.
      On the functions, mechanisms, and malfunctions of intracortical contextual modulation.
      ). Predictive coding conceives of the brain as a hierarchy whose goal is to maximize the evidence for its model of the world by comparing prior beliefs with sensory data, and using the resultant prediction errors (PEs) to update the model (Figure 1). Model evidence can also be maximized through active inference—that is, by acting on the world (and thus selecting sensory evidence) to minimize PEs (
      • Friston K.
      The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory?.
      ). Moreover, hierarchical Bayesian inference entails modeling ourselves as agents who change the world: indeed, in this scheme, experiences such as agency and selfhood are inferred from the consequences of our own actions (
      • Sterzer P.
      • Mishara A.L.
      • Voss M.
      • Heinz A.
      Thought insertion as a self-disturbance: An integration of predictive coding and phenomenological approaches.
      ).
      Figure thumbnail gr1
      Figure 1Schematic illustration of Bayesian predictive coding as an explanatory framework for psychosis. (A) Predictions are encoded at higher levels of a hierarchical system and are sent as predictive signals to lower levels (downward arrows on the left). Whenever the incoming sensory data violate these predictions, a prediction error signal is sent to update the predictive model at higher levels (upward arrow on the right). Formalized as Bayesian inference, predictions (prior) and sensory data (likelihood) are represented in the form of probability distributions. The posterior results from the combination of prior and likelihood according to Bayes’ rule, weighted by their respective precisions π (which is the inverse of their variance σ; see first equation), and updates the predictive model (third equation). The fourth equation rearranges the third to show that the new posterior mean is simply the old prior mean added to a precision-weighted prediction error. (B) In psychosis, the balance between predictions and sensory data has been proposed to be disrupted, with a decreased precision in the representation of priors and increased precision of the likelihood
      (
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      )
      . This imbalance biases Bayesian inference toward the likelihood and away from the prior, resulting in the abnormally strong weighting of prediction error. Candidate mechanisms for decreased prior and increased likelihood precisions are hypofunction of glutamatergic N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDA-Rs) and increased dopamine (DA) activity, respectively. Some psychotic phenomena may be explained by a compensatory increase in feedback signaling at higher levels of the hierarchy (bold arrow, upper left).
      In terms of neural implementation (
      • Bastos A.M.
      • Usrey W.M.
      • Adams R.A.
      • Mangun G.R.
      • Fries P.
      • Friston K.J.
      Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding.
      ,
      • Shipp S.
      Neural elements for predictive coding.
      ), predictive signals may be sent from higher hierarchical levels predominantly via glutamatergic N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) signaling; any disparity between prior belief and sensory data is then signaled as a PE to the higher levels, mostly via glutamatergic alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid receptors. Animal and human studies of vision support this hypothesis (
      • Ekstrom A.D.
      • Kahana M.J.
      • Caplan J.B.
      • Fields T.A.
      • Isham E.A.
      • Newman E.L.
      • et al.
      Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation.
      ,
      • Self M.W.
      • Kooijmans R.N.
      • Super H.
      • Lamme V.A.
      • Roelfsema P.R.
      Different glutamate receptors convey feedforward and recurrent processing in macaque V1.
      ,
      • Roelfsema P.R.
      • Lamme V.A.
      • Spekreijse H.
      • Bosch H.
      Figure-ground segregation in a recurrent network architecture.
      ,
      • Summerfield C.
      • de Lange F.P.
      Expectation in perceptual decision making: Neural and computational mechanisms.
      ). In Bayesian terms, the PE corresponds to the difference between the means of the prior and the likelihood distributions and is weighted by their relative precisions (
      • Mathys C.
      • Daunizeau J.
      • Friston K.J.
      • Stephan K.E.
      A Bayesian foundation for individual learning under uncertainty.
      ), whereby precision corresponds to the inverse variances of their respective probability distributions (Figure 1A). Roughly, this can be thought of as the relative reliability of priors or sensory data, the extent to which each colors current inference and learning by weighting the impact of PEs (
      • Mathys C.
      • Daunizeau J.
      • Friston K.J.
      • Stephan K.E.
      A Bayesian foundation for individual learning under uncertainty.
      ). Precision is thought to be signaled by neuromodulators such as dopamine and acetylcholine, depending on the particular inferential hierarchy (
      • Fiorillo C.D.
      • Newsome W.T.
      • Schultz W.
      The temporal precision of reward prediction in dopamine neurons.
      ,
      • Galea J.M.
      • Bestmann S.
      • Beigi M.
      • Jahanshahi M.
      • Rothwell J.C.
      Action reprogramming in Parkinson's disease: Response to prediction error is modulated by levels of dopamine.
      ,
      • Iglesias S.
      • Mathys C.
      • Brodersen K.H.
      • Kasper L.
      • Piccirelli M.
      • den Ouden H.E.
      • et al.
      Hierarchical prediction errors in midbrain and basal forebrain during sensory learning.
      ). Perturbations in these neuromodulators are thus candidates for the profound departures from consensual reality that characterize psychotic states (
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis.
      ).
      Functional magnetic resonance imaging has shown that feedback from higher- to lower-level sensory cortices carries spatiotemporally precise and context-specific predictions (
      • Harrison L.M.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Rees G.
      • Friston K.J.
      Extra-classical receptive field effects measured in striate cortex with fMRI.
      ,
      • Alink A.
      • Schwiedrzik C.M.
      • Kohler A.
      • Singer W.
      • Muckli L.
      Stimulus predictability reduces responses in primary visual cortex.
      ,
      • Sterzer P.
      • Haynes J.D.
      • Rees G.
      Primary visual cortex activation on the path of apparent motion is mediated by feedback from hMT+/V5.
      ,
      • Vetter P.
      • Smith F.W.
      • Muckli L.
      Decoding sound and imagery content in early visual cortex.
      ,
      • Muckli L.
      • De Martino F.
      • Vizioli L.
      • Petro L.S.
      • Smith F.W.
      • Ugurbil K.
      • et al.
      Contextual feedback to superficial layers of V1.
      ). When predictions are confirmed by sensory input, this leads to a dampening of neural responses (
      • Kok P.
      • de Lange F.P.
      Shape perception simultaneously up- and downregulates neural activity in the primary visual cortex.
      ,
      • Kok P.
      • Brouwer G.J.
      • van Gerven M.A.
      • de Lange F.P.
      Prior expectations bias sensory representations in visual cortex.
      ), while violation of predictions leads to enhanced responses compatible with PE signaling (
      • Alink A.
      • Schwiedrzik C.M.
      • Kohler A.
      • Singer W.
      • Muckli L.
      Stimulus predictability reduces responses in primary visual cortex.
      ). Electrophysiological studies investigating neural responses to deviant stimuli, such as the mismatch negativity, suggest a hierarchical organization of prediction and PE signaling (
      • Wacongne C.
      • Labyt E.
      • van Wassenhove V.
      • Bekinschtein T.
      • Naccache L.
      • Dehaene S.
      Evidence for a hierarchy of predictions and prediction errors in human cortex.
      ,
      • Wacongne C.
      A predictive coding account of MMN reduction in schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Lieder F.
      • Daunizeau J.
      • Garrido M.I.
      • Friston K.J.
      • Stephan K.E.
      Modelling trial-by-trial changes in the mismatch negativity.
      ). In the time-frequency domain, oscillatory signals have been related to predictive coding, with feedback signaling of predictions being mediated predominantly by the alpha/beta frequency bands and feedforward PE signaling by gamma-band activity (
      • Bastos A.M.
      • Usrey W.M.
      • Adams R.A.
      • Mangun G.R.
      • Fries P.
      • Friston K.J.
      Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding.
      ,
      • Arnal L.H.
      • Giraud A.-L.
      Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions.
      ,
      • Michalareas G.
      • Vezoli J.
      • van Pelt S.
      • Schoffelen J.M.
      • Kennedy H.
      • Fries P.
      Alpha-beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas.
      ,
      • Bastos A.M.
      • Vezoli J.
      • Bosman C.A.
      • Schoffelen J.M.
      • Oostenveld R.
      • Dowdall J.R.
      • et al.
      Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels.
      ).
      There is a deep relevance of this account to psychosis, in terms of both neurobiology (glutamatergic and dopaminergic systems in schizophrenia, acetylcholine in hallucinosis) and phenomenology (perception, beliefs, agency, and ipseity). We now outline previous theories of psychosis that are highly relevant to the predictive coding account (
      • Gray J.A.
      On biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia.
      ).

      Precursor Theories of Predictive Coding

      Anticipating the focus of predictive coding accounts on perceptual inference, and in line with phenomenological observations (
      • Mayer-Gross W.
      Die Klinik der Schizophrenie.
      ,
      • Matussek P.
      [Studies on delusional perception. I. Changes of the perceived external world in incipient primary delusion].
      ,
      • Uhlhaas P.J.
      • Mishara A.L.
      Perceptual anomalies in schizophrenia: Integrating phenomenology and cognitive neuroscience.
      ), early theories of psychosis emphasized altered perception. Maher (
      • Maher B.A.
      Delusional thinking and perceptual disorder.
      ) highlighted the failure to integrate sensory input with learned expectations, which was further developed by Gray et al. (
      • Gray J.A.
      • Feldon J.
      • Rawlins J.N.P.
      • Hemsley D.
      • Smith D.A.
      The neuropsychology of schizophrenia.
      ) and Hemsley and Garety (
      • Hemsley D.R.
      • Garety P.A.
      The formation of maintenance of delusions: A Bayesian analysis.
      ). Hemsley and Garety (
      • Hemsley D.R.
      • Garety P.A.
      The formation of maintenance of delusions: A Bayesian analysis.
      ) put forth the first explicitly Bayesian analysis of delusions, suggesting how belief, evidence, and their disrupted interaction could garner aberrant inference. Hemsley and Garety (
      • Hemsley D.R.
      • Garety P.A.
      The formation of maintenance of delusions: A Bayesian analysis.
      ) and Gray et al. (
      • Gray J.A.
      • Feldon J.
      • Rawlins J.N.P.
      • Hemsley D.
      • Smith D.A.
      The neuropsychology of schizophrenia.
      ) argued that perception proceeded through modeling of the world and that neural signals normally evoked by surprising events are inappropriately engaged in psychosis. As a consequence, patients attend to and learn about events that others would ignore, forming the grounds for both hallucinations and delusions. A similar idea was later developed in the wake of fundamental discoveries regarding the role of dopamine in motivational salience and reward PE signaling (
      • Schultz W.
      • Dayan P.
      • Montague P.R.
      A neural substrate of prediction and reward.
      ,
      • Berridge K.C.
      • Robinson T.E.
      What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?.
      ). Heinz (
      • Heinz A.
      Dopaminergic dysfunction in alcoholism and schizophrenia–psychopathological and behavioral correlates.
      ) and Kapur (
      • Kapur S.
      Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia.
      ) proposed that excessive dopamine signaling results in a misattribution of salience to normally inconspicuous events, which then demand explanation, culminating in delusions. Another influential theory of psychosis, the comparator model, suggested impaired predictive signaling as a key mechanism underlying hallucinations (
      • Feinberg I.
      Efference copy and corollary discharge: Implications for thinking and its disorders.
      ) and later so-called passivity phenomena, such as the experience of one’s actions or thoughts being externally controlled (
      • Frith C.D.
      • Done D.J.
      Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action.
      ). The comparator model proposes a failure to predict one’s own actions owing to impaired corollary discharge, which normally serves to predict and explain away the sensory consequences of self-initiated actions. Later versions suggested that the consequences of any action are predicted by a neural forward model (
      • Wolpert D.M.
      • Ghahramani Z.
      • Jordan M.I.
      An internal model for sensorimotor integration.
      ) and that it is the reduced precision of these predictions that leads to the experience of alien control (
      • Sterzer P.
      • Mishara A.L.
      • Voss M.
      • Heinz A.
      Thought insertion as a self-disturbance: An integration of predictive coding and phenomenological approaches.
      ,
      • Synofzik M.
      • Vosgerau G.
      • Voss M.
      The experience of agency: An interplay between prediction and postdiction.
      ).
      Most of these models focused on one specific symptom dimension. However, the above-chance co-occurrence of a number of characteristic phenomena in psychotic disorders demands theories that can accommodate multiple symptoms. Moreover, most earlier theories failed to integrate the multitude of documented neurobiological abnormalities and focused on one particular mechanism while disregarding others. For instance, while the idea of salience misattribution related delusions primarily to dopamine dysfunction, more recent accounts along these lines have provided a broader picture by outlining how dopaminergic dysfunction may be linked to altered glutamatergic and gamma-aminobutyric acidergic neurotransmission (
      • Heinz A.
      • Schlagenhauf F.
      Dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: Salience attribution revisited.
      ,
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Honey G.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      Prediction error, ketamine and psychosis: An updated model.
      ). Meanwhile, neurocognitive theories have made advances largely at the conceptual level. Empirical tests of these theories could yield evidence for a theory or against it but could not provide quantitative, mechanistic evidence. Predictive coding can provide such mechanistic evidence by estimating model parameters at the level of the individual (
      • Wang X.J.
      • Krystal J.H.
      Computational psychiatry.
      ,
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Mathys C.
      Computational approaches to psychiatry.
      ,
      • Huys Q.J.
      • Maia T.V.
      • Frank M.J.
      Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications.
      ), and relating those parameters to the severity and type of psychotic symptoms.

      A Predictive Coding Account of Psychosis

      In Bayesian predictive coding schemes, the PE is affected by the precision of the sensory data: if it is high, the precision-weighted PE in case of a mismatch will be greater, and vice versa (Figure 1A)—just as in classical statistical inference, the t statistic is greater if the standard error of the data is smaller. Furthermore, the degree to which a prior belief will change in response to a PE is also determined by its own precision: an imprecise prior will update more than a precise one will. It is crucial to represent accurately the precisions of both prior beliefs and sensory data, as a failure to do so will lead to false inferences (just as overestimating the precision of the data causes type I errors). Psychosis has been related to a decreased precision of prior beliefs and/or increased precision of sensory data (
      • Sterzer P.
      • Mishara A.L.
      • Voss M.
      • Heinz A.
      Thought insertion as a self-disturbance: An integration of predictive coding and phenomenological approaches.
      ,
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis.
      ,
      • Fletcher P.C.
      • Frith C.D.
      Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ,
      • Friston K.J.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Montague R.
      • Dolan R.J.
      Computational psychiatry: The brain as a phantastic organ.
      ,
      • Notredame C.E.
      • Pins D.
      • Deneve S.
      • Jardri R.
      What visual illusions teach us about schizophrenia.
      ). This imbalance in precisions shifts the posterior toward the sensory data and away from the prior (Figure 1B), and inference is thus driven more strongly by the sensory data.
      This notion, which we here refer to as canonical predictive coding account of psychosis, is supported by several lines of evidence. For example, psychosis has been associated with a greater resistance to visual illusions (which rely on prior beliefs for their effects), a failure to attenuate sensory consequences of self-generated actions, impaired smooth visual pursuit of a moving target, but improved tracking of unpredictable changes in target motion, a decreased influence of stimulus predictability on brain responses [e.g., N400, P300, mismatch negativity; but see Erickson et al. (
      • Erickson M.A.
      • Albrecht M.
      • Ruffle A.
      • Fleming L.
      • Corlett P.
      • Gold J.
      No association between symptom severity and MMN impairment in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic approach.
      )], and a loss of corticothalamic connectivity [for reviews, see Adams et al. (
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ) and Notredame et al. (
      • Notredame C.E.
      • Pins D.
      • Deneve S.
      • Jardri R.
      What visual illusions teach us about schizophrenia.
      )]. The main neurotransmitter alterations that are thought to underlie this predictive coding abnormality are hypofunction of cortical NMDARs and gamma-aminobutyric acidergic neurons as well as elevated striatal dopamine D2 receptor activity, as reviewed elsewhere (
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis.
      ,
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ,
      • Jardri R.
      • Hugdahl K.
      • Hughes M.
      • Brunelin J.
      • Waters F.
      • Alderson-Day B.
      • et al.
      Are hallucinations due to an imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory influences on the brain?.
      ). The resulting aberrant encoding of precision could lead to an abnormally strong weighting of PEs, which in turn leads to aberrant learning and the formation of delusional beliefs (
      • Heinz A.
      • Schlagenhauf F.
      Dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: Salience attribution revisited.
      ,
      • Fletcher P.C.
      • Frith C.D.
      Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ,
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Honey G.D.
      • Krystal J.H.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      Glutamatergic model psychoses: Prediction error, learning, and inference.
      ). This canonical predictive coding account of psychosis is not without controversy. Some frank psychotic symptoms have been related to increased prior precision and therefore a stronger impact of prior beliefs. We return to this issue below.
      One strength of predictive coding is that it is more generalized than earlier accounts, which tended to localize the pathology to a specific brain area or psychological function, e.g., the pathway connecting the subiculum to the nucleus accumbens (
      • Gray J.A.
      • Feldon J.
      • Rawlins J.N.P.
      • Hemsley D.
      • Smith D.A.
      The neuropsychology of schizophrenia.
      ), striatal dopamine release (
      • Heinz A.
      Dopaminergic dysfunction in alcoholism and schizophrenia–psychopathological and behavioral correlates.
      ,
      • Kapur S.
      Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia.
      ), or altered corollary discharge (
      • Frith C.D.
      • Done D.J.
      Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action.
      ). By providing a generic framework compatible with previous neurocognitive theories and neurobiological data, predictive coding also holds promise of accounting for more than one psychotic symptom. It provides a plausible explanation not only for delusional mood and paranoid delusions, akin to the aberrant salience account (
      • Heinz A.
      Dopaminergic dysfunction in alcoholism and schizophrenia–psychopathological and behavioral correlates.
      ,
      • Kapur S.
      Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia.
      ), but also for hallucinations (
      • Notredame C.E.
      • Pins D.
      • Deneve S.
      • Jardri R.
      What visual illusions teach us about schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Jardri R.
      • Hugdahl K.
      • Hughes M.
      • Brunelin J.
      • Waters F.
      • Alderson-Day B.
      • et al.
      Are hallucinations due to an imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory influences on the brain?.
      ) and passivity phenomena (
      • Sterzer P.
      • Mishara A.L.
      • Voss M.
      • Heinz A.
      Thought insertion as a self-disturbance: An integration of predictive coding and phenomenological approaches.
      ,
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ). On the predictive coding view, corollary discharge becomes a prediction of the sensory consequences of action. A failure of that prediction renders those consequences surprising, garnering the inference that actions were under external control rather than self-authored.
      While predictive coding thus has the potential to unify accounts of psychosis (
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ) and integrate empirical evidence at different levels of observation and within a formal quantitative model, a number of important challenges remain.

      The Heterogeneity of Psychosis

      The heterogeneity of psychosis and the fact that delusions and hallucinations co-occur, but to varying degrees, demands explanation. However, an overly flexible or general theory that explains everything will be of little use. In our view, predictive coding puts forward a skeletal understanding of how, given a perturbation to a component of the model, the phenomenological outcome has particular characteristics. In other words, predictive coding does not reduce psychosis to a single cause, but rather attempts to show how different underlying pathophysiologies could perturb the system in ways that produce overlapping phenomenologies.
      This challenge is exemplified by arguments as to whether a single deficit within a predictive coding model can explain both perceptual and cognitive aspects of psychotic symptoms. The two-factor account (
      • Coltheart M.
      • Langdon R.
      • McKay R.
      Schizophrenia and monothematic delusions.
      ) invokes both perceptual and cognitive problems in the genesis of some delusions, based on the observation of both abnormal percepts and bizarre explanations of these percepts. According to predictive coding, reduced precision of priors could potentially account for both factors, given that it would alter perceptual inference and make cognitive explanations for altered percepts less constrained (
      • Fletcher P.C.
      • Frith C.D.
      Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia.
      ). Recent neurobiological work, however, has raised the question of whether a loss of prior precision (e.g., prefrontal hypoconnectivity) and gain in sensory precision (e.g., sensory hyperconnectivity) may indeed be two separate factors in the illness (
      • Javitt D.C.
      • Sweet R.A.
      Auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia: Integrating clinical and basic features.
      ,
      • Clementz B.A.
      • Sweeney J.
      • Keshavan M.S.
      • Pearlson G.
      • Tamminga C.A.
      Using biomarker batteries.
      ). These observations might be reconciled by adding some nuance to the single-layer predictive coding example outlined above. Predictive coding actually takes place across large multilevel hierarchies in which the precision weighting of PEs may be controlled—at least in part—independently at different levels and in different sensory modalities (
      • Friston K.
      • Kiebel S.
      Predictive coding under the free-energy principle.
      ). Thus, NMDAR (or other neuromodulatory) dysfunction may have widespread and diverse effects on the precision of prior beliefs in perceptual and cognitive domains. Furthermore, NMDAR-mediated interneuron dysfunction may not only disinhibit (i.e., amplify) sensory areas, but also reduce the stability of more sustained representations in higher areas (i.e., reduce the signal-to-noise ratio), leading to increased sensory and decreased prior precision, respectively.
      A recent study emphasized the importance of analyzing the different weightings of priors that may be implemented at different hierarchical levels. The authors probed the use of prior knowledge to perceive the gist versus the details of ambiguous images in a healthy population with varying degrees of hallucination and delusion proneness (
      • Davies D.J.
      • Teufel C.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      Anomalous perceptions and beliefs are associated with shifts toward different types of prior knowledge in perceptual inference.
      ). Hallucination proneness correlated with stronger employment of global (gist) and local (detail) priors, whereas delusion proneness was associated with less reliance on local priors. This raises a hitherto underappreciated mechanism through which the heterogeneity in psychotic phenomenology could be explained, namely differential weightings of specific hierarchical levels in different psychotic symptoms (
      • Kwisthout J.
      • Bekkering H.
      • van Rooij I.
      To be precise, the details don't matter: On predictive processing, precision, and level of detail of predictions.
      ). The neural circuits and neurochemical mechanisms of these effects ought to be established. Where to draw the line between perceptual and conceptual processing remains a challenge, and indeed, whether and how high-level prior beliefs modulate perceptual processes is controversial (
      • Firestone C.
      • Scholl B.J.
      Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for "top-down" effects.
      ). However, recent neural data suggest that they do (
      • Schmack K.
      • Gomez-Carrillo de Castro A.
      • Rothkirch M.
      • Sekutowicz M.
      • Rossler H.
      • Haynes J.D.
      • et al.
      Delusions and the role of beliefs in perceptual inference.
      ,
      • Samaha J.
      • Boutonnet B.
      • Postle B.R.
      • Lupyan G.
      Effects of meaningfulness on perception: Alpha-band oscillations carry perceptual expectations and influence early visual responses.
      ), and that the impact of priors on perception may be enhanced in those with hallucinations (
      • Powers A.R.
      • Mathys C.
      • Corlett P.R.
      Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors.
      ,
      • Cassidy C.M.
      • Balsam P.D.
      • Weinstein J.J.
      • Rosengard R.J.
      • Slifstein M.
      • Daw N.D.
      • et al.
      A perceptual inference mechanism for hallucinations linked to striatal dopamine.
      ,
      • Alderson-Day B.
      • Lima C.F.
      • Evans S.
      • Krishnan S.
      • Shanmugalingam P.
      • Fernyhough C.
      • et al.
      Distinct processing of ambiguous speech in people with non-clinical auditory verbal hallucinations.
      ).

      Hallucinations: Strong or Weak Priors, or Both?

      Hallucinations represent a challenge, as two apparently opposing aberrations have been proposed and there is evidence supporting both. One view has linked hallucinations to a failure to attenuate sensory precision, including the sensory consequences of inner speech, analogous to the mechanism that is thought to underlie delusions of control (
      • Fletcher P.C.
      • Frith C.D.
      Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Nazimek J.M.
      • Hunter M.D.
      • Woodruff P.W.
      Auditory hallucinations: Expectation-perception model.
      ,
      • Allen P.
      • Aleman A.
      • McGuire P.K.
      Inner speech models of auditory verbal hallucinations: Evidence from behavioural and neuroimaging studies.
      ,
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Friston K.J.
      • Frith C.D.
      Dysconnection in schizophrenia: From abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring.
      ,
      • van Lutterveld R.
      • Sommer I.E.
      • Ford J.M.
      The neurophysiology of auditory hallucinations – a historical and contemporary review.
      ). This would correspond to the notion of low precision of priors relative to a disproportionately high precision of neural signals that encode inner speech in auditory cortex, akin to the canonical predictive coding account. Indeed, hallucination severity in patients with schizophrenia is associated with a failure to attenuate predictable signals in the somatosensory cortex (
      • Shergill S.S.
      • White T.P.
      • Joyce D.W.
      • Bays P.M.
      • Wolpert D.M.
      • Frith C.D.
      Functional magnetic resonance imaging of impaired sensory prediction in schizophrenia.
      ). Similarly, a model-based functional magnetic resonance imaging study using probabilistic presentation of speech stimuli found diminished auditory cortex PE-related activations and deactivations to the unexpected presence or absence of speech, respectively, in patients with hallucinations, suggesting aberrant PE signaling (
      • Horga G.
      • Schatz K.C.
      • Abi-Dargham A.
      • Peterson B.S.
      Deficits in predictive coding underlie hallucinations in schizophrenia.
      ).
      Alternatively, hallucinations may result from enhanced rather than weakened top-down predictive signaling (i.e., increased precision of priors) on neural activity in sensory cortices (
      • Friston K.
      Hallucinations and perceptual inference.
      ). Perception would therefore rely less on the sensory input and more on beliefs. Supporting this notion, directional bottom-up connectivity from Wernicke’s to Broca’s areas is reduced in individuals who hear voices (
      • Curcic-Blake B.
      • Liemburg E.
      • Vercammen A.
      • Swart M.
      • Knegtering H.
      • Bruggeman R.
      • et al.
      When Broca goes uninformed: Reduced information flow to Broca's area in schizophrenia patients with auditory hallucinations.
      ). Top-down predictions from Broca’s area may thus be less constrained by sensory information. Recently, people who hear voices were found to be more susceptible to conditioning-induced hallucinations, and accordingly, modeling in a Bayesian framework showed stronger perceptual priors (
      • Powers A.R.
      • Mathys C.
      • Corlett P.R.
      Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors.
      ). Another recent study investigated the perception of auditory stimuli under different levels of uncertainty (
      • Cassidy C.M.
      • Balsam P.D.
      • Weinstein J.J.
      • Rosengard R.J.
      • Slifstein M.
      • Daw N.D.
      • et al.
      A perceptual inference mechanism for hallucinations linked to striatal dopamine.
      ). Hallucinations in schizophrenia patients correlated with a perceptual bias that reflected increased weighting of prior beliefs. This bias could be pharmacologically induced by amphetamine and strongly correlated with striatal dopamine release. Together, these findings favor a strong-prior account of hallucinations and thus call into question the suggestion that aberrant salience of inner speech confers the content of voices.
      How can these apparently contradictory findings be reconciled? The auditory system may have a strong prior for speech—perhaps because this is a highly salient signal for our species—and as such, noisy signals in the auditory cortex are resolved by that prior into perceived speech (akin to our propensity to see faces in clouds, for example). At the same time, corollary discharge (i.e., descending predictions regarding the consequences of action) may still have a role, in ascribing agency to those experiences. In this case, disruption of corollary discharge as a form of predictive signaling may be more broadly relevant for both hallucinations and delusions, which entail aberrant inferences about both agency and the intentions of others. This may explain the lack of specificity of corollary discharge deficits to specific positive symptoms (
      • Ford J.M.
      • Mathalon D.H.
      Anticipating the future: Automatic prediction failures in schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Thakkar K.N.
      • Diwadkar V.A.
      • Rolfs M.
      Oculomotor prediction: A window into the psychotic mind.
      ).
      Furthermore, priors at low and high hierarchical levels may be differentially affected. Neurobiologically, this may be mediated by the higher density of recurrent connections in higher-level association cortices, compared with primary sensory regions, such that a psychotogenic perturbation that impacts excitatory/inhibitory (E/I) balance may have more profound effects higher rather than lower in the hierarchy (
      • Yang G.J.
      • Murray J.D.
      • Wang X.J.
      • Glahn D.C.
      • Pearlson G.D.
      • Repovs G.
      • et al.
      Functional hierarchy underlies preferential connectivity disturbances in schizophrenia.
      ) [see Jardri and Deneve (
      • Jardri R.
      • Deneve S.
      Circular inferences in schizophrenia.
      ) for a detailed exposition of the role of E/I balance in learning, inference, and psychosis]. In brief, the E/I relationships may implement exactly the predictive cancellation mechanisms that underlie predictive coding. Blocking NMDARs (with ketamine for example) profoundly alters E/I balance (
      • Murray J.D.
      • Anticevic A.
      • Gancsos M.
      • Ichinose M.
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Krystal J.H.
      • et al.
      Linking microcircuit dysfunction to cognitive impairment: Effects of disinhibition associated with schizophrenia in a cortical working memory model.
      ,
      • Anticevic A.
      • Gancsos M.
      • Murray J.D.
      • Repovs G.
      • Driesen N.R.
      • Ennis D.J.
      • et al.
      NMDA receptor function in large-scale anticorrelated neural systems with implications for cognition and schizophrenia.
      ), thus altering the balance between priors and PEs (
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis.
      ), perhaps differently at different hierarchical levels (
      • Yang G.J.
      • Murray J.D.
      • Wang X.J.
      • Glahn D.C.
      • Pearlson G.D.
      • Repovs G.
      • et al.
      Functional hierarchy underlies preferential connectivity disturbances in schizophrenia.
      ). Many findings in psychotic or psychosis-prone individuals point to weak priors that are implemented at low levels [e.g., visual illusions; see above and (
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis.
      ,
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ,
      • Jardri R.
      • Hugdahl K.
      • Hughes M.
      • Brunelin J.
      • Waters F.
      • Alderson-Day B.
      • et al.
      Are hallucinations due to an imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory influences on the brain?.
      )]. Impaired predictive coding at low levels may result in perceptual uncertainties that may be (partly) compensated by reliance on high-level abstract or semantic prior beliefs (Figure 1B). This may result in a top-down enhancement of signals in sensory cortices, thus facilitating hallucinations. There are even data suggesting that psychotic individuals with and without hallucinations utilize different priors to different extents in the same task. Powers et al. (
      • Powers A.R.
      • Mathys C.
      • Corlett P.R.
      Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors.
      ) found that people with hallucinations had strong perceptual priors that were not present in psychotic patients who did not hallucinate and who, indeed, may have had weak priors. The presence of strong priors and their immunity to updating were associated with strong insula and hippocampal responses, respectively (
      • Powers A.R.
      • Mathys C.
      • Corlett P.R.
      Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors.
      ). These psychological and circuit observations should be replicated, manipulated with transcranial magnetic stimulation (
      • Taylor J.J.
      • Krystal J.H.
      • D'Souza D.C.
      • Gerrard J.L.
      • Corlett P.R.
      Targeted neural network interventions for auditory hallucinations: Can TMS inform DBS?.
      ) or real-time neurofeedback (
      • Orlov N.D.
      • Giampietro V.
      • O'Daly O.
      • Lam S.L.
      • Barker G.J.
      • Rubia K.
      • et al.
      Real-time fMRI neurofeedback to down-regulate superior temporal gyrus activity in patients with schizophrenia and auditory hallucinations: A proof-of-concept study.
      ), and the mediating role of glutamate and E/I balance at different hierarchical levels should be explored in human pharmacological and patient studies as well as animal models.

      Changes in Psychotic Phenomenology Over Time

      Another important challenge for theories of psychosis is that the pathophysiology may change over the course of the underlying disorder (
      • Insel T.R.
      Rethinking schizophrenia.
      ). While changes of symptomatology over time were emphasized by phenomenologists (
      • Conrad K.
      Die beginnende Schizophrenie. Versuch einer Gestaltanalyse des Wahns.
      ,
      • Mishara A.L.
      • Corlett P.R.
      Are delusions biologically adaptive? Salvaging the doxastic shear pin.
      ), they are largely neglected by current classification systems. For example, delusions are often highly fixed and incorrigible in chronic patients, while they are still malleable in early psychosis (
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis.
      ). With time and treatment, they may become less impactful on function. Thus, the underlying pathophysiology may also change over time and differentially contribute to psychopathology at different stages of illness. Evidence from magnetic resonance spectroscopy suggests that alterations in glutamatergic neurotransmission may change over the course of schizophrenia (
      • Marsman A.
      • van den Heuvel M.P.
      • Klomp D.W.
      • Kahn R.S.
      • Luijten P.R.
      • Hulshoff Pol H.E.
      Glutamate in schizophrenia: A focused review and meta-analysis of (1)H-MRS studies.
      ,
      • Merritt K.
      • Egerton A.
      • Kempton M.J.
      • Taylor M.J.
      • McGuire P.K.
      Nature of glutamate alterations in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies.
      ). Indeed, ketamine infusion in healthy volunteers may better mimic the E/I dysbalance and hierarchical perturbations observed in first-episode patients than in those with more chronic illness (
      • Anticevic A.
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Cole M.W.
      • Savic A.
      • Gancsos M.
      • Tang Y.
      • et al.
      N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist effects on prefrontal cortical connectivity better model early than chronic schizophrenia.
      ). We note with interest that the metabotropic glutamate agonist pomaglumetad appears to have efficacy in early rather than chronic schizophrenia, suggesting that hyperglutamatergia is more involved around the onset and early phases of illness (
      • Anticevic A.
      • Hu X.
      • Xiao Y.
      • Hu J.
      • Li F.
      • Bi F.
      • et al.
      Early-course unmedicated schizophrenia patients exhibit elevated prefrontal connectivity associated with longitudinal change.
      ,
      • Kinon B.J.
      • Millen B.A.
      • Zhang L.
      • McKinzie D.L.
      Exploratory analysis for a targeted patient population responsive to the metabotropic glutamate 2/3 receptor agonist pomaglumetad methionil in schizophrenia.
      ). The issue is further complicated by the possibility that such changes over time are not limited to aspects of brain development and learning, but rather involve ongoing neurobiological and environmental influences, including effects of antipsychotic medication and drug use. Current data are consistent with the idea that with chronicity, prefrontal glutamate signaling may progress from an excess to an insufficiency. Future work with magnetic resonance spectroscopy and electrophysiological markers of E/I balance could track these changes and pinpoint their effects on predictive coding (
      • Weilnhammer V.
      • Stuke H.
      • Sterzer P.
      • Schmack K.
      The neural correlates of hierarchical predictions for perceptual decisions.
      ). More broadly, in predictive coding, the brain is involved in a dynamic prediction-based negotiation with the world, which evolves as the person tries out new models of reality. While they eventually settle on beliefs that become engrained, one would expect the patient's priors to evolve across time.

      The Persistence of Psychotic Experiences

      An important unresolved question is how aberrant predictive coding might account not only for the emergence of delusions, but also for their persistence. It is a defining feature of delusions that they persist despite contradicting evidence. This suggests an excessive influence of delusional beliefs on the perception of new information [e.g., (
      • Jaspers K.
      Allgmeine Psychopathologie, 9th ed.
      )], which would entail an increased precision of delusion-related priors. In contrast, the emergence of delusions might result from decreased precision of priors as outlined above (
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis.
      ,
      • Fletcher P.C.
      • Frith C.D.
      Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ) (Figure 1B). Evidence from experiments using the NMDAR antagonist ketamine, which has been previously shown to induce aberrant PEs, suggests a link between PE signaling and memory reconsolidation, which could strengthen delusional beliefs and foster their persistence (
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Cambridge V.
      • Gardner J.M.
      • Piggot J.S.
      • Turner D.C.
      • Everitt J.C.
      • et al.
      Ketamine effects on memory reconsolidation favor a learning model of delusions.
      ,
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Krystal J.H.
      • Taylor J.R.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      Why do delusions persist?.
      ). An additional (or complementary) mechanism could be related to an imbalance between priors at low and high levels of the predictive coding hierarchy, as suggested by a series of studies investigating perceptual inference in relation to delusions (
      • Schmack K.
      • Gomez-Carrillo de Castro A.
      • Rothkirch M.
      • Sekutowicz M.
      • Rossler H.
      • Haynes J.D.
      • et al.
      Delusions and the role of beliefs in perceptual inference.
      ,
      • Schmack K.
      • Rothkirch M.
      • Priller J.
      • Sterzer P.
      Enhanced predictive signalling in schizophrenia.
      ,
      • Schmack K.
      • Schnack A.
      • Priller J.
      • Sterzer P.
      Perceptual instability in schizophrenia: Probing predictive coding accounts of delusions with ambiguous stimuli.
      ,
      • Schmack K.
      • Rossler H.
      • Sekutowicz M.
      • Brandl E.J.
      • Muller D.J.
      • Petrovic P.
      • et al.
      Linking unfounded beliefs to genetic dopamine availability.
      ). In contrast to weak low-level priors, the effects of more abstract high-level priors may be abnormally strong (Figure 1B). Such a mechanism could sculpt perception into conformity with delusional beliefs and thus contribute to their persistence. An increased influence of learned high-level beliefs in relation to psychotic symptoms was also reported for the perception of images with impoverished sensory information where perceptual inference relies strongly on priors (
      • Teufel C.
      • Subramaniam N.
      • Dobler V.
      • Perez J.
      • Finnemann J.
      • Mehta P.R.
      • et al.
      Shift toward prior knowledge confers a perceptual advantage in early psychosis and psychosis-prone healthy individuals.
      ). Differential roles of priors at low and high levels of the hierarchy are also suggested by recent evidence relating delusion proneness to reduced usage of prior beliefs in perceptual but not cognitive decision making (
      • Stuke H.
      • Weilnhammer V.A.
      • Sterzer P.
      • Schmack K.
      Delusion proneness is linked to a reduced usage of prior beliefs in perceptual decisions.
      ).
      Furthermore, aberrant predictive coding could render other people unreliable, to be treated with suspicion. This could account for the social content of psychotic symptoms, but may also explain why they persist, and even strengthen, in the face of efforts to refute them (
      • Griffin J.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      Predictive processing, source monitoring, and psychosis.
      ). Perceptible social cues may be more uncertain than nonsocial ones, because they may or may not serve as reliable signals of others’ intentions, which we can never fully know (
      • Biedermann F.
      • Frajo-Apor B.
      • Hofer A.
      Theory of mind and its relevance in schizophrenia.
      ). Consequently, high-level social priors may be particularly influential in the perception and beliefs of those with psychotic symptoms (
      • Griffin J.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      Predictive processing, source monitoring, and psychosis.
      ). There may also be a motivated quality to psychotic inferences (
      • Fineberg S.K.
      • Corlett P.R.
      The doxastic shear pin: Delusions as errors of learning and memory.
      ). That is, psychotic symptoms may provide a form of personal identity, and personal-level data may be assumed to be more reliable than those from others. Finally, beliefs have value in and of themselves. Psychotic symptoms may be seen as attempts to garner some advantage, perhaps by convincing others of their veracity (
      • Fineberg S.K.
      • Corlett P.R.
      The doxastic shear pin: Delusions as errors of learning and memory.
      ). This renders them susceptible to the same biases and asymmetries in updating observed with nondelusional beliefs (
      • Fineberg S.K.
      • Corlett P.R.
      The doxastic shear pin: Delusions as errors of learning and memory.
      ). These asymmetries can be explained with a Bayesian model, if we allow agents to derive utility from their beliefs (
      • Sharot T.
      • Garrett N.
      Forming beliefs: Why valence matters.
      ).

      A Roadmap for Future Research

      Predictive coding was not conceived to explain psychosis. It is a general theory of brain function. If it is a useful theory of how the brain works, then it should also be useful to account for states of aberrant brain function such as psychosis. The question is therefore not whether there is one specific abnormality in predictive coding that can explain psychosis, but rather whether predictive coding provides a framework that can help us to better understand psychosis. We believe that its greatest strength is that it can be formulated in computational terms and therefore lends itself to rigorous quantitative testing. However, while there is abundant empirical evidence compatible with a predictive coding account, more research is needed that explicitly tests (and potentially falsifies) predictions derived from this theory. We therefore advocate research that addresses the outlined challenges head-on, in a hypothesis-driven way, and with the methodological rigor that is provided by the computational framework.
      One key question that has received too little attention relates to the hierarchical nature of predictive coding. Potentially different roles of high and low levels of the hierarchy were highlighted throughout our discussion of important challenges to predictive coding. Such differences may resolve apparent inconsistencies regarding weak versus strong priors, help to understand the heterogeneity in the phenomenology of psychosis, and explain changes in symptomatology over time. Table 1 summarizes the theory and controversy regarding the predictive coding alterations underlying hallucinations and delusions. Experimental tasks are needed that reliably pinpoint predictive coding at low versus high levels of the hierarchy. Such tasks could then be used in conjunction with computational modeling [for a recent example, see Weilnhammer et al. (
      • Weilnhammer V.
      • Stuke H.
      • Sterzer P.
      • Schmack K.
      The neural correlates of hierarchical predictions for perceptual decisions.
      )] to directly test, e.g., the hypothesis that delusions are related to weak low-level priors and hallucinations are related to strong high-level priors.
      Table 1Predictive Coding and Positive Symptoms: Theory and Controversy
      SymptomFeatureTheoryLiteratureControversy
      HallucinationsPercepts without external stimulusStrong perceptual priorsPowers et al.
      • Powers 3rd, A.R.
      • Kelley M.
      • Corlett P.R.
      Hallucinations as top-down effects on perception.
      Entails weak and strong prior beliefs—for perception and action—in the same brain at the same time
      Speech from external agentsWeak corollary dischargeThakkar et al.
      • Thakkar K.N.
      • Diwadkar V.A.
      • Rolfs M.
      Oculomotor prediction: A window into the psychotic mind.
      DelusionsDelusional mood/aberrant salienceWeak perceptual priorsCorlett et al.
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Taylor J.R.
      • Wang X.J.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      • Krystal J.H.
      Toward a neurobiology of delusions.
      Necessitates a transition from weak to strong priors as delusions form, foment, and become ingrained
      Fixed in the face of contradictory evidenceStrong memory reconsolidation/strong conceptual priorsCorlett et al.
      • Corlett P.R.
      • Krystal J.H.
      • Taylor J.R.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      Why do delusions persist?.
      ; Schmack et al.
      • Schmack K.
      • Gomez-Carrillo de Castro A.
      • Rothkirch M.
      • Sekutowicz M.
      • Rossler H.
      • Haynes J.D.
      • et al.
      Delusions and the role of beliefs in perceptual inference.
      Here we highlight the facets of hallucinations and delusions that have been addressed by predictive coding–based theories. Each has garnered empirical support; however, overarching theories—grounded in a broader multisensory and enactive framework that can accommodate the evolution and trajectories of positive symptoms—are required. We focus here on hallucinations and delusions. For consideration of other psychotic symptoms such as thought disorder and passivity phenomena from the viewpoint of predictive coding, please see Griffin and Fletcher
      • Griffin J.D.
      • Fletcher P.C.
      Predictive processing, source monitoring, and psychosis.
      and Sterzer et al.
      • Sterzer P.
      • Mishara A.L.
      • Voss M.
      • Heinz A.
      Thought insertion as a self-disturbance: An integration of predictive coding and phenomenological approaches.
      .
      Another important direction will be research into neural markers of hierarchical feedback and feedforward processing and their relation to the precisions of prior beliefs and PEs, respectively, in Bayesian inference. Recent advances in the neuroimaging of laminar anatomical projection patterns will help in this regard (
      • Muckli L.
      • De Martino F.
      • Vizioli L.
      • Petro L.S.
      • Smith F.W.
      • Ugurbil K.
      • et al.
      Contextual feedback to superficial layers of V1.
      ,
      • Michalareas G.
      • Vezoli J.
      • van Pelt S.
      • Schoffelen J.M.
      • Kennedy H.
      • Fries P.
      Alpha-beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas.
      ). Computational modeling should be used to examine how precision is reflected in neural measurements, and rigorous state-of-the-art model comparison is needed to probe predictive coding against other models of message passing. Pharmacological models are a promising approach to probe the roles of candidate neurotransmitter systems. Their direct comparison with neural predictive coding alterations in relation to specific psychotic symptom dimensions will help to address key challenges in psychosis research, such as the phenomenological heterogeneity of psychosis. Animal models should be further developed into an additional pillar of psychosis research, as important insights are expected from a more targeted manipulation of specific brain circuits and transmitter systems. For example, optogenetic manipulation of E/I balance (
      • Yizhar O.
      • Fenno L.E.
      • Prigge M.
      • Schneider F.
      • Davidson T.J.
      • O'Shea D.J.
      • et al.
      Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction.
      ) could be used to explore the computations underlying predictive coding. Similarly, models that identify how genes relate to brain development (
      • Hardingham G.E.
      • Do K.Q.
      Linking early-life NMDAR hypofunction and oxidative stress in schizophrenia pathogenesis.
      ) and changes the canonical microcircuits (
      • Bastos A.M.
      • Usrey W.M.
      • Adams R.A.
      • Mangun G.R.
      • Fries P.
      • Friston K.J.
      Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding.
      ) involved in aberrant predictive coding are warranted. Ideally, different levels of investigation should be translationally integrated within a common computational modeling framework.
      At the level of symptoms, we need a better understanding of the processes underlying specific psychotic symptoms and their interrelationships. For example, delusion- and hallucination-related processes should be investigated at the same time in the same patients to examine how these neural and symptom processes are organized. Intriguing epidemiological data suggest a hierarchy from hallucinations to delusions (
      • Minas I.H.
      • Stuart G.W.
      • Klimidis S.
      • Jackson H.J.
      • Singh B.S.
      • Copolov D.L.
      Positive and negative symptoms in the psychoses: Multidimensional scaling of SAPS and SANS items.
      ). Indeed, what we learn about these processes should be applied at the level of diagnostic entities, with a number of possible implications. First, understanding the predictive coding mechanisms underlying psychosis may lead to the delineation of new entities within and across existing diagnostic groups such as schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Second, models are needed that distinguish psychosis from other psychiatric syndromes. For example, current models of autism are strikingly similar to the predictive coding account of psychosis (
      • Pellicano E.
      • Burr D.
      When the world becomes 'too real': A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception.
      ,
      • van Schalkwyk G.I.
      • Volkmar F.R.
      • Corlett P.R.
      A predictive coding account of psychotic symptoms in autism spectrum disorder.
      ,
      • Palmer C.J.
      • Lawson R.P.
      • Hohwy J.
      Bayesian approaches to autism: Towards volatility, action, and behavior.
      ). Future investigations should examine differences and commonalities in neural computations in individuals with psychosis and those with autism (
      • van Schalkwyk G.I.
      • Volkmar F.R.
      • Corlett P.R.
      A predictive coding account of psychotic symptoms in autism spectrum disorder.
      ). Third, predictive coding can relate psychosis to “normal” brain function, which may help to destigmatize the disorder (
      • Insel T.R.
      Rethinking schizophrenia.
      ): psychosis may be understood as a variety of brain function, in line with the so-called continuum view (
      • van Os J.
      • Reininghaus U.
      Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population.
      ), which considers psychotic symptoms as extreme expressions of normal traits. “False inferences” made by psychotic individuals may be rendered comprehensible given the premises of predictive coding. As Adams et al. (
      • Adams R.A.
      • Stephan K.E.
      • Brown H.R.
      • Frith C.D.
      • Friston K.J.
      The computational anatomy of psychosis.
      ) stated, “From the point of view of the subject its inferences are Bayes-optimal. It is only our attribution of the inference as false that gives it an illusory or delusionary aspect.”
      Importantly, a more complete model of psychosis may help patients understand their experiences, which could aid the development of psychotherapies. Moreover, predictive coding offers the possibility of more specific and quantitative predictions about symptoms and their mechanisms. Such an approach may in turn help not only to use those drugs that are currently available in a more targeted way, but also to develop new pharmacological interventions.

      Acknowledgments and Disclosures

      This work was supported by German Research Foundation Grant Nos. STE 1430/6-2 (to PS) and STE 1430/7-1 (to PS); Academy of Medical Sciences Grant No. AMS-SGCL13-Adams (to RAA); National Institute of Health Research Grant No. CL-2013-18-003 (to RAA); Wellcome Trust Grant No. WT095692MA (to PF); the Bernard Wolfe Health Neuroscience Fund (to PF); European Research Council Grant No. ERC StG 2012_311751 (Brain reading of contextual feedback and predictions) (to LM); European Union Horizon Research and Innovation Programme Grant No. 720270 (HBPSGAI) (to LM); the Swedish Research Council (to PP); the Marianne och Marcus Wallenberg Foundation (to PP); the Swedish Brain Foundation (to PP); the Stockholm County Council (to PP); the Karolinska Institute (to PP); Lundbeck Foundation (to PU); Lilly (to PU); the Connecticut Mental Health Center and Connecticut State Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services (to PRC); National Institute of Mental Health Grant Nos. R01MH112887 (to PRC) and 5R01MH067073-09 (to PRC); International Mental Health Review Order/Janssen Rising Star Translational Research Award (to PRC); National Center for Research Resources and National Center for Advancing Translational Science Clinical and Translational Science Award Grant No. UL1 TR000142 (to PRC); the National Institutes of Health (to PRC); the National Institutes of Health Roadmap for Medical Research (to PRC); and the Clinical Neurosciences Division of the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs, National Center for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorders, Veterans Affairs Connecticut Healthcare System (West Haven, CT). The contents of this work are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official view of National Institutes of Health or the Connecticut Mental Health Center/Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services.
      In the past 3 years, SML has received personal fees from Janssen and Sunovion. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

      References

        • Schennach R.
        • Riedel M.
        • Obermeier M.
        • Spellmann I.
        • Musil R.
        • Jager M.
        • et al.
        What are residual symptoms in schizophrenia spectrum disorder? Clinical description and 1-year persistence within a naturalistic trial.
        Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2015; 265: 107-116
        • Togay B.
        • Noyan H.
        • Tasdelen R.
        • Ucok A.
        Clinical variables associated with suicide attempts in schizophrenia before and after the first episode.
        Psychiatry Res. 2015; 229: 252-256
        • Maia T.V.
        • Frank M.J.
        An integrative perspective on the role of dopamine in schizophrenia.
        Biol Psychiatry. 2017; 81: 52-66
        • Rao R.P.
        • Ballard D.H.
        Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects.
        Nat Neurosci. 1999; 2: 79-87
        • Friston K.J.
        A theory of cortical responses.
        Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005; 360: 815-836
        • Clark A.
        Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science.
        Behav Brain Sci. 2013; 36: 181-204
        • von Helmholtz H.
        Handbuch der physiologischen Optik.
        Leopold Voss, Leipzig, Germany1867
        • Lee T.S.
        • Mumford D.
        Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex.
        J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2003; 20: 1434-1448
        • Hohwy J.
        Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain.
        Front Psychol. 2012; 3: 96
        • Jardri R.
        • Deneve S.
        Circular inferences in schizophrenia.
        Brain. 2013; 136: 3227-3241
        • Phillips W.A.
        • Clark A.
        • Silverstein S.M.
        On the functions, mechanisms, and malfunctions of intracortical contextual modulation.
        Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015; 52: 1-20
        • Friston K.
        The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory?.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010; 11: 127-138
        • Sterzer P.
        • Mishara A.L.
        • Voss M.
        • Heinz A.
        Thought insertion as a self-disturbance: An integration of predictive coding and phenomenological approaches.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2016; 10: 502
        • Bastos A.M.
        • Usrey W.M.
        • Adams R.A.
        • Mangun G.R.
        • Fries P.
        • Friston K.J.
        Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding.
        Neuron. 2012; 76: 695-711
        • Shipp S.
        Neural elements for predictive coding.
        Front Psychol. 2016; 7: 1792
        • Ekstrom A.D.
        • Kahana M.J.
        • Caplan J.B.
        • Fields T.A.
        • Isham E.A.
        • Newman E.L.
        • et al.
        Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation.
        Nature. 2003; 425: 184-188
        • Self M.W.
        • Kooijmans R.N.
        • Super H.
        • Lamme V.A.
        • Roelfsema P.R.
        Different glutamate receptors convey feedforward and recurrent processing in macaque V1.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012; 109: 11031-11036
        • Roelfsema P.R.
        • Lamme V.A.
        • Spekreijse H.
        • Bosch H.
        Figure-ground segregation in a recurrent network architecture.
        J Cogn Neurosci. 2002; 14: 525-537
        • Summerfield C.
        • de Lange F.P.
        Expectation in perceptual decision making: Neural and computational mechanisms.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014; 15: 745-756
        • Mathys C.
        • Daunizeau J.
        • Friston K.J.
        • Stephan K.E.
        A Bayesian foundation for individual learning under uncertainty.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2011; 5: 39
        • Fiorillo C.D.
        • Newsome W.T.
        • Schultz W.
        The temporal precision of reward prediction in dopamine neurons.
        Nat Neurosci. 2008; 11: 966-973
        • Galea J.M.
        • Bestmann S.
        • Beigi M.
        • Jahanshahi M.
        • Rothwell J.C.
        Action reprogramming in Parkinson's disease: Response to prediction error is modulated by levels of dopamine.
        J Neurosci. 2012; 32: 542-550
        • Iglesias S.
        • Mathys C.
        • Brodersen K.H.
        • Kasper L.
        • Piccirelli M.
        • den Ouden H.E.
        • et al.
        Hierarchical prediction errors in midbrain and basal forebrain during sensory learning.
        Neuron. 2013; 80: 519-530
        • Corlett P.R.
        • Frith C.D.
        • Fletcher P.C.
        From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis.
        Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009; 206: 515-530
        • Harrison L.M.
        • Stephan K.E.
        • Rees G.
        • Friston K.J.
        Extra-classical receptive field effects measured in striate cortex with fMRI.
        Neuroimage. 2007; 34: 1199-1208
        • Alink A.
        • Schwiedrzik C.M.
        • Kohler A.
        • Singer W.
        • Muckli L.
        Stimulus predictability reduces responses in primary visual cortex.
        J Neurosci. 2010; 30: 2960-2966
        • Sterzer P.
        • Haynes J.D.
        • Rees G.
        Primary visual cortex activation on the path of apparent motion is mediated by feedback from hMT+/V5.
        Neuroimage. 2006; 32: 1308-1316
        • Vetter P.
        • Smith F.W.
        • Muckli L.
        Decoding sound and imagery content in early visual cortex.
        Curr Biol. 2014; 24: 1256-1262
        • Muckli L.
        • De Martino F.
        • Vizioli L.
        • Petro L.S.
        • Smith F.W.
        • Ugurbil K.
        • et al.
        Contextual feedback to superficial layers of V1.
        Curr Biol. 2015; 25: 2690-2695
        • Kok P.
        • de Lange F.P.
        Shape perception simultaneously up- and downregulates neural activity in the primary visual cortex.
        Curr Biol. 2014; 24: 1531-1535
        • Kok P.
        • Brouwer G.J.
        • van Gerven M.A.
        • de Lange F.P.
        Prior expectations bias sensory representations in visual cortex.
        J Neurosci. 2013; 33: 16275-16284
        • Wacongne C.
        • Labyt E.
        • van Wassenhove V.
        • Bekinschtein T.
        • Naccache L.
        • Dehaene S.
        Evidence for a hierarchy of predictions and prediction errors in human cortex.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011; 108: 20754-20759
        • Wacongne C.
        A predictive coding account of MMN reduction in schizophrenia.
        Biol Psychol. 2016; 116: 68-74
        • Lieder F.
        • Daunizeau J.
        • Garrido M.I.
        • Friston K.J.
        • Stephan K.E.
        Modelling trial-by-trial changes in the mismatch negativity.
        PLoS Comput Biol. 2013; 9: e1002911
        • Arnal L.H.
        • Giraud A.-L.
        Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions.
        Trends Cogn Sci. 2012; 16: 390-398
        • Michalareas G.
        • Vezoli J.
        • van Pelt S.
        • Schoffelen J.M.
        • Kennedy H.
        • Fries P.
        Alpha-beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas.
        Neuron. 2016; 89: 384-397
        • Bastos A.M.
        • Vezoli J.
        • Bosman C.A.
        • Schoffelen J.M.
        • Oostenveld R.
        • Dowdall J.R.
        • et al.
        Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels.
        Neuron. 2015; 85: 390-401
        • Gray J.A.
        On biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia.
        Am J Psychiatry. 2004; 161 (author reply 377–378): 376
        • Mayer-Gross W.
        Die Klinik der Schizophrenie.
        in: Bumke O. Handbuch der Geisteskrankheiten. Springer, Berlin1932: 293-578
        • Matussek P.
        [Studies on delusional perception. I. Changes of the perceived external world in incipient primary delusion].
        Archiv Psychiatr Nervenkr Z Gesamte Neurol und Psychiatr. 1952; 189 (contd): 279-319
        • Uhlhaas P.J.
        • Mishara A.L.
        Perceptual anomalies in schizophrenia: Integrating phenomenology and cognitive neuroscience.
        Schizophr Bull. 2007; 33: 142-156
        • Maher B.A.
        Delusional thinking and perceptual disorder.
        J Indiv Psychol. 1974; 30: 98-113
        • Gray J.A.
        • Feldon J.
        • Rawlins J.N.P.
        • Hemsley D.
        • Smith D.A.
        The neuropsychology of schizophrenia.
        Behav Brain Sci. 1991; 14: 1-20
        • Hemsley D.R.
        • Garety P.A.
        The formation of maintenance of delusions: A Bayesian analysis.
        Br J Psychiatry. 1986; 149: 51-56
        • Schultz W.
        • Dayan P.
        • Montague P.R.
        A neural substrate of prediction and reward.
        Science. 1997; 275: 1593-1599
        • Berridge K.C.
        • Robinson T.E.
        What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?.
        Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998; 28: 309-369
        • Heinz A.
        Dopaminergic dysfunction in alcoholism and schizophrenia–psychopathological and behavioral correlates.
        Eur Psychiatry. 2002; 17: 9-16
        • Kapur S.
        Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia.
        Am J Psychiatry. 2003; 160: 13-23
        • Feinberg I.
        Efference copy and corollary discharge: Implications for thinking and its disorders.
        Schizophr Bull. 1978; 4: 636-640
        • Frith C.D.
        • Done D.J.
        Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action.
        Psychol Med. 1989; 19: 359-363
        • Wolpert D.M.
        • Ghahramani Z.
        • Jordan M.I.
        An internal model for sensorimotor integration.
        Science. 1995; 269: 1880-1882
        • Synofzik M.
        • Vosgerau G.
        • Voss M.
        The experience of agency: An interplay between prediction and postdiction.
        Front Psychol. 2013; 4: 127
        • Heinz A.
        • Schlagenhauf F.
        Dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: Salience attribution revisited.
        Schizophr Bull. 2010; 36: 472-485
        • Corlett P.R.
        • Honey G.D.
        • Fletcher P.C.
        Prediction error, ketamine and psychosis: An updated model.
        J Psychopharmacol. 2016; 30: 1145-1155
        • Wang X.J.
        • Krystal J.H.
        Computational psychiatry.
        Neuron. 2014; 84: 638-654
        • Stephan K.E.
        • Mathys C.
        Computational approaches to psychiatry.
        Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014; 25: 85-92
        • Huys Q.J.
        • Maia T.V.
        • Frank M.J.
        Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications.
        Nat Neurosci. 2016; 19: 404-413
        • Fletcher P.C.
        • Frith C.D.
        Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009; 10: 48-58
        • Adams R.A.
        • Stephan K.E.
        • Brown H.R.
        • Frith C.D.
        • Friston K.J.
        The computational anatomy of psychosis.
        Front Psychiatry. 2013; 4: 47
        • Friston K.J.
        • Stephan K.E.
        • Montague R.
        • Dolan R.J.
        Computational psychiatry: The brain as a phantastic organ.
        Lancet Psychiatry. 2014; 1: 148-158
        • Notredame C.E.
        • Pins D.
        • Deneve S.
        • Jardri R.
        What visual illusions teach us about schizophrenia.
        Front Integr Neurosci. 2014; 8: 63
        • Erickson M.A.
        • Albrecht M.
        • Ruffle A.
        • Fleming L.
        • Corlett P.
        • Gold J.
        No association between symptom severity and MMN impairment in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic approach.
        Schizophr Res Cogn. 2017; 9: 13-17
        • Jardri R.
        • Hugdahl K.
        • Hughes M.
        • Brunelin J.
        • Waters F.
        • Alderson-Day B.
        • et al.
        Are hallucinations due to an imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory influences on the brain?.
        Schizophr Bull. 2016; 42: 1124-1134
        • Corlett P.R.
        • Honey G.D.
        • Krystal J.H.
        • Fletcher P.C.
        Glutamatergic model psychoses: Prediction error, learning, and inference.
        Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011; 36: 294-315
        • Coltheart M.
        • Langdon R.
        • McKay R.
        Schizophrenia and monothematic delusions.
        Schizophr Bull. 2007; 33: 642-647
        • Javitt D.C.
        • Sweet R.A.
        Auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia: Integrating clinical and basic features.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015; 16: 535-550
        • Clementz B.A.
        • Sweeney J.
        • Keshavan M.S.
        • Pearlson G.
        • Tamminga C.A.
        Using biomarker batteries.
        Biol Psychiatry. 2015; 77: 90-92
        • Friston K.
        • Kiebel S.
        Predictive coding under the free-energy principle.
        Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009; 364: 1211-1221
        • Davies D.J.
        • Teufel C.
        • Fletcher P.C.
        Anomalous perceptions and beliefs are associated with shifts toward different types of prior knowledge in perceptual inference.
        Schizophr Bull. 2017; ([published online ahead of print Dec 27])
        • Kwisthout J.
        • Bekkering H.
        • van Rooij I.
        To be precise, the details don't matter: On predictive processing, precision, and level of detail of predictions.
        Brain Cogn. 2017; 112: 84-91
        • Firestone C.
        • Scholl B.J.
        Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for "top-down" effects.
        Behav Brain Sci. 2016; 39: e229
        • Schmack K.
        • Gomez-Carrillo de Castro A.
        • Rothkirch M.
        • Sekutowicz M.
        • Rossler H.
        • Haynes J.D.
        • et al.
        Delusions and the role of beliefs in perceptual inference.
        J Neurosci. 2013; 33: 13701-13712
        • Samaha J.
        • Boutonnet B.
        • Postle B.R.
        • Lupyan G.
        Effects of meaningfulness on perception: Alpha-band oscillations carry perceptual expectations and influence early visual responses.
        Sci Rep. 2018; 8: 6606
        • Powers A.R.
        • Mathys C.
        • Corlett P.R.
        Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors.
        Science. 2017; 357: 596-600
        • Cassidy C.M.
        • Balsam P.D.
        • Weinstein J.J.
        • Rosengard R.J.
        • Slifstein M.
        • Daw N.D.
        • et al.
        A perceptual inference mechanism for hallucinations linked to striatal dopamine.
        Curr Biol. 2018; 28: 503-514.e4
        • Alderson-Day B.
        • Lima C.F.
        • Evans S.
        • Krishnan S.
        • Shanmugalingam P.
        • Fernyhough C.
        • et al.
        Distinct processing of ambiguous speech in people with non-clinical auditory verbal hallucinations.
        Brain. 2017; 140: 2475-2489
        • Nazimek J.M.
        • Hunter M.D.
        • Woodruff P.W.
        Auditory hallucinations: Expectation-perception model.
        Med Hypotheses. 2012; 78: 802-810
        • Allen P.
        • Aleman A.
        • McGuire P.K.
        Inner speech models of auditory verbal hallucinations: Evidence from behavioural and neuroimaging studies.
        Int Rev Psychiatry. 2007; 19: 407-415
        • Stephan K.E.
        • Friston K.J.
        • Frith C.D.
        Dysconnection in schizophrenia: From abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring.
        Schizophr Bull. 2009; 35: 509-527
        • van Lutterveld R.
        • Sommer I.E.
        • Ford J.M.
        The neurophysiology of auditory hallucinations – a historical and contemporary review.
        Front Psychiatry. 2011; 2: 28
        • Shergill S.S.
        • White T.P.
        • Joyce D.W.
        • Bays P.M.
        • Wolpert D.M.
        • Frith C.D.
        Functional magnetic resonance imaging of impaired sensory prediction in schizophrenia.
        JAMA Psychiatry. 2014; 71: 28-35
        • Horga G.
        • Schatz K.C.
        • Abi-Dargham A.
        • Peterson B.S.
        Deficits in predictive coding underlie hallucinations in schizophrenia.
        J Neurosci. 2014; 34: 8072-8082
        • Friston K.
        Hallucinations and perceptual inference.
        Behav Brain Sci. 2005; 28: 764-766
        • Curcic-Blake B.
        • Liemburg E.
        • Vercammen A.
        • Swart M.
        • Knegtering H.
        • Bruggeman R.
        • et al.
        When Broca goes uninformed: Reduced information flow to Broca's area in schizophrenia patients with auditory hallucinations.
        Schizophr Bull. 2013; 39: 1087-1095
        • Ford J.M.
        • Mathalon D.H.
        Anticipating the future: Automatic prediction failures in schizophrenia.
        Int J Psychophysiol. 2012; 83: 232-239
        • Thakkar K.N.
        • Diwadkar V.A.
        • Rolfs M.
        Oculomotor prediction: A window into the psychotic mind.
        Trends Cogn Sci. 2017; 21: 344-356
        • Yang G.J.
        • Murray J.D.
        • Wang X.J.
        • Glahn D.C.
        • Pearlson G.D.
        • Repovs G.
        • et al.
        Functional hierarchy underlies preferential connectivity disturbances in schizophrenia.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016; 113: E219-E228
        • Murray J.D.
        • Anticevic A.
        • Gancsos M.
        • Ichinose M.
        • Corlett P.R.
        • Krystal J.H.
        • et al.
        Linking microcircuit dysfunction to cognitive impairment: Effects of disinhibition associated with schizophrenia in a cortical working memory model.
        Cereb Cortex. 2014; 24: 859-872
        • Anticevic A.
        • Gancsos M.
        • Murray J.D.
        • Repovs G.
        • Driesen N.R.
        • Ennis D.J.
        • et al.
        NMDA receptor function in large-scale anticorrelated neural systems with implications for cognition and schizophrenia.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012; 109: 16720-16725
        • Taylor J.J.
        • Krystal J.H.
        • D'Souza D.C.
        • Gerrard J.L.
        • Corlett P.R.
        Targeted neural network interventions for auditory hallucinations: Can TMS inform DBS?.
        Schizophr Res. 2017; 195: 455-462
        • Orlov N.D.
        • Giampietro V.
        • O'Daly O.
        • Lam S.L.
        • Barker G.J.
        • Rubia K.
        • et al.
        Real-time fMRI neurofeedback to down-regulate superior temporal gyrus activity in patients with schizophrenia and auditory hallucinations: A proof-of-concept study.
        Transl Psychiatry. 2018; 8: 46
        • Insel T.R.
        Rethinking schizophrenia.
        Nature. 2010; 468: 187-193
        • Conrad K.
        Die beginnende Schizophrenie. Versuch einer Gestaltanalyse des Wahns.
        Thieme, Stuttgart, Germany1959
        • Mishara A.L.
        • Corlett P.R.
        Are delusions biologically adaptive? Salvaging the doxastic shear pin.
        Behav Brain Sci. 2009; 32: 530-531
        • Marsman A.
        • van den Heuvel M.P.
        • Klomp D.W.
        • Kahn R.S.
        • Luijten P.R.
        • Hulshoff Pol H.E.
        Glutamate in schizophrenia: A focused review and meta-analysis of (1)H-MRS studies.
        Schizophr Bull. 2013; 39: 120-129
        • Merritt K.
        • Egerton A.
        • Kempton M.J.
        • Taylor M.J.
        • McGuire P.K.
        Nature of glutamate alterations in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies.
        JAMA Psychiatry. 2016; 73: 665-674
        • Anticevic A.
        • Corlett P.R.
        • Cole M.W.
        • Savic A.
        • Gancsos M.
        • Tang Y.
        • et al.
        N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist effects on prefrontal cortical connectivity better model early than chronic schizophrenia.
        Biol Psychiatry. 2015; 77: 569-580
        • Anticevic A.
        • Hu X.
        • Xiao Y.
        • Hu J.
        • Li F.
        • Bi F.
        • et al.
        Early-course unmedicated schizophrenia patients exhibit elevated prefrontal connectivity associated with longitudinal change.
        J Neurosci. 2015; 35: 267-286
        • Kinon B.J.
        • Millen B.A.
        • Zhang L.
        • McKinzie D.L.
        Exploratory analysis for a targeted patient population responsive to the metabotropic glutamate 2/3 receptor agonist pomaglumetad methionil in schizophrenia.
        Biol Psychiatry. 2015; 78: 754-762
        • Weilnhammer V.
        • Stuke H.
        • Sterzer P.
        • Schmack K.
        The neural correlates of hierarchical predictions for perceptual decisions.
        J Neurosci. 2018; 38: 5008-5021
        • Jaspers K.
        Allgmeine Psychopathologie, 9th ed.
        Springer, Berlin1973
        • Corlett P.R.
        • Cambridge V.
        • Gardner J.M.
        • Piggot J.S.
        • Turner D.C.
        • Everitt J.C.
        • et al.
        Ketamine effects on memory reconsolidation favor a learning model of delusions.
        PLoS One. 2013; 8: e65088
        • Corlett P.R.
        • Krystal J.H.
        • Taylor J.R.
        • Fletcher P.C.
        Why do delusions persist?.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2009; 3: 12
        • Schmack K.
        • Rothkirch M.
        • Priller J.
        • Sterzer P.
        Enhanced predictive signalling in schizophrenia.
        Hum Brain Mapp. 2017; 38: 1767-1779
        • Schmack K.
        • Schnack A.
        • Priller J.
        • Sterzer P.
        Perceptual instability in schizophrenia: Probing predictive coding accounts of delusions with ambiguous stimuli.
        Schizophr Res Cogn. 2015; 2: 72-77
        • Schmack K.
        • Rossler H.
        • Sekutowicz M.
        • Brandl E.J.
        • Muller D.J.
        • Petrovic P.
        • et al.
        Linking unfounded beliefs to genetic dopamine availability.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2015; 9: 521
        • Teufel C.
        • Subramaniam N.
        • Dobler V.
        • Perez J.
        • Finnemann J.
        • Mehta P.R.
        • et al.
        Shift toward prior knowledge confers a perceptual advantage in early psychosis and psychosis-prone healthy individuals.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015; 112: 13401-13406
        • Stuke H.
        • Weilnhammer V.A.
        • Sterzer P.
        • Schmack K.
        Delusion proneness is linked to a reduced usage of prior beliefs in perceptual decisions.
        Schizophr Bull. 2018; ([published online ahead of print Jan 20])
        • Griffin J.D.
        • Fletcher P.C.
        Predictive processing, source monitoring, and psychosis.
        Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017; 13: 265-289
        • Biedermann F.
        • Frajo-Apor B.
        • Hofer A.
        Theory of mind and its relevance in schizophrenia.
        Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2012; 25: 71-75
        • Fineberg S.K.
        • Corlett P.R.
        The doxastic shear pin: Delusions as errors of learning and memory.
        Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2016; 21: 73-89
        • Sharot T.
        • Garrett N.
        Forming beliefs: Why valence matters.
        Trends Cogn Sci. 2016; 20: 25-33
        • Yizhar O.
        • Fenno L.E.
        • Prigge M.
        • Schneider F.
        • Davidson T.J.
        • O'Shea D.J.
        • et al.
        Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction.
        Nature. 2011; 477: 171-178
        • Hardingham G.E.
        • Do K.Q.
        Linking early-life NMDAR hypofunction and oxidative stress in schizophrenia pathogenesis.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016; 17: 125-134
        • Minas I.H.
        • Stuart G.W.
        • Klimidis S.
        • Jackson H.J.
        • Singh B.S.
        • Copolov D.L.
        Positive and negative symptoms in the psychoses: Multidimensional scaling of SAPS and SANS items.
        Schizophr Res. 1992; 8: 143-156
        • Pellicano E.
        • Burr D.
        When the world becomes 'too real': A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception.
        Trends Cogn Sci. 2012; 16: 504-510
        • van Schalkwyk G.I.
        • Volkmar F.R.
        • Corlett P.R.
        A predictive coding account of psychotic symptoms in autism spectrum disorder.
        J Autism Dev Disord. 2017; 47: 1323-1340
        • Palmer C.J.
        • Lawson R.P.
        • Hohwy J.
        Bayesian approaches to autism: Towards volatility, action, and behavior.
        Psychol Bull. 2017; 143: 521-542
        • van Os J.
        • Reininghaus U.
        Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population.
        World Psychiatry. 2016; 15: 118-124
        • Powers 3rd, A.R.
        • Kelley M.
        • Corlett P.R.
        Hallucinations as top-down effects on perception.
        Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2016; 1: 393-400
        • Corlett P.R.
        • Taylor J.R.
        • Wang X.J.
        • Fletcher P.C.
        • Krystal J.H.
        Toward a neurobiology of delusions.
        Prog Neurobiol. 2010; 92: 345-369