Advertisement
Commentary| Volume 64, ISSUE 12, P1024-1025, December 15, 2008

Dissecting the Biology of Prefrontal Cortical Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: Deficiency in Mnemonic Processing

  • Graham V. Williams
    Correspondence
    Address reprint requests to Graham V. Williams, D.Phil., Yale University School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, 300 George St, Suite 901, New Haven, CT 06511
    Affiliations
    Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven; and VA Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, Connecticut
    Search for articles by this author
  • Stacy A. Castner
    Affiliations
    Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven; and VA Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, Connecticut
    Search for articles by this author
      Alterations in the normal recruitment of neuronal activity within prefrontal cortex (PFC) are directly related to changes in performance on cognitive tasks. Such alterations have particularly deleterious consequences for working memory, which has been considered to comprise the core cognitive deficit in schizophrenia and may prove to be the best predictor of outcome in patients. Neuroimaging studies promise to reveal important facets of altered prefrontal neuronal activity in multiple neuropsychiatric disorders in relation to different aspects of cognitive function. Elucidating the alterations in neuronal activity in relation to working memory processes dramatically enhances our capability to shed new insights into the biological mechanisms that are involved in prefrontal dysfunction. In their article in this issue, Driesen et al. (
      • Driesen N.R.
      • Leung H.C.
      • Calhoun V.D.
      • Constable R.T.
      • Gueorguieva R.
      • Hoffman R.
      • et al.
      Impairment of working memory maintenance and response in schizophrenia: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence.
      ) squarely target the ability to maintain neuronal activity in particular regions of PFC as a core deficiency in prefrontal function in patients as compared with healthy control subjects performing a working memory task. This finding was made all the more significant by utilizing a task based on the knowledge gained from many years of research in the nonhuman primate. From the earliest studies in the monkey, it has been observed that many neurons in PFC fire during the delay period of delayed response tasks (
      • Fuster J.M.
      • Alexander G.E.
      Neuron activity related to short-term memory.
      ,
      • Funahashi S.
      • Bruce C.J.
      • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
      Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
      ). As this delay activity appears to be selective for one memorandum or target location in the task and not another, it has been hypothesized to provide a cellular substrate for representation of a stimulus to be remembered, i.e., representational memory (
      • Daigneault S.
      • Braün C.M.
      • Whitaker H.A.
      An empirical test of two opposing theoretical models of prefrontal function.
      ). In this issue, Driesen et al. (
      • Driesen N.R.
      • Leung H.C.
      • Calhoun V.D.
      • Constable R.T.
      • Gueorguieva R.
      • Hoffman R.
      • et al.
      Impairment of working memory maintenance and response in schizophrenia: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence.
      ) were able to examine the human parallel of this delay activity in the brains of patients with schizophrenia by employing a task in which the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response could be examined throughout and segregated into encoding, maintenance, and response phases of the working memory process. Capitalizing on a spatial delayed response task developed by Leung et al. (
      • Leung H.C.
      • Gore J.C.
      • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
      Sustained mnemonic response in the human middle frontal gyrus during on-line storage of spatial memoranda.
      ), they used a “cue” period wherein two or four spatial stimuli were presented followed by a long delay period of 16 seconds before presentation of the probe stimulus that required subjects to make a judgment as to whether it was a match or nonmatch to the locations of any of the cue stimuli. This paradigm allowed for a distinction between cue, delay, and response-related changes in the BOLD signal and identification of the impact of working memory load on these changes in patients as compared with control subjects. By these means, it became possible to resolve the hemodynamic response into an encoding peak, maintenance activity, and a response peak, much like the cue, delay, and response activity recorded in area 46 of nonhuman primates performing oculomotor delayed response tasks (
      • Funahashi S.
      • Bruce C.J.
      • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
      Visuospatial coding in primate prefrontal neurons revealed by oculomotor paradigms.
      ). By elevating working memory load or increasing the length of the delay period in their study, Leung et al. (
      • Leung H.C.
      • Gore J.C.
      • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
      Sustained mnemonic response in the human middle frontal gyrus during on-line storage of spatial memoranda.
      ) pinpointed the role of middle frontal gyrus (MFG; Brodmann areas 46/9) in the maintenance component of working memory processing, at least within the spatial domain. This finding provided direct evidence for conservation of the role of dorsolateral PFC, particularly area 46, in the maintenance of a neural representation of spatial information across human and nonhuman primates.
      To read this article in full you will need to make a payment

      Purchase one-time access:

      Academic & Personal: 24 hour online accessCorporate R&D Professionals: 24 hour online access
      One-time access price info
      • For academic or personal research use, select 'Academic and Personal'
      • For corporate R&D use, select 'Corporate R&D Professionals'

      Subscribe:

      Subscribe to Biological Psychiatry
      Already a print subscriber? Claim online access
      Already an online subscriber? Sign in
      Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect

      References

        • Driesen N.R.
        • Leung H.C.
        • Calhoun V.D.
        • Constable R.T.
        • Gueorguieva R.
        • Hoffman R.
        • et al.
        Impairment of working memory maintenance and response in schizophrenia: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence.
        Biol Psychiatry. 2008; 64: 1026-1034
        • Fuster J.M.
        • Alexander G.E.
        Neuron activity related to short-term memory.
        Science. 1971; 173: 652-654
        • Funahashi S.
        • Bruce C.J.
        • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
        Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
        J Neurophysiol. 1989; 61: 331-349
        • Daigneault S.
        • Braün C.M.
        • Whitaker H.A.
        An empirical test of two opposing theoretical models of prefrontal function.
        Brain Cogn. 1992; 19: 48-71
        • Leung H.C.
        • Gore J.C.
        • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
        Sustained mnemonic response in the human middle frontal gyrus during on-line storage of spatial memoranda.
        J Cogn Neurosci. 2002; 14: 659-671
        • Funahashi S.
        • Bruce C.J.
        • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
        Visuospatial coding in primate prefrontal neurons revealed by oculomotor paradigms.
        J Neurophysiol. 1990; 63: 814-831
        • Lisman J.E.
        • Fellous J.M.
        • Wang X.J.
        A role for NMDA-receptor channels in working memory.
        Nat Neurosci. 1998; 1: 273-275
        • Krystal J.H.
        • Anand A.
        • Moghaddam B.
        Effects of NMDA receptor antagonists: Implications for the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.
        Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002; 59: 663-664
        • Williams G.V.
        • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
        Modulation of memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex.
        Nature. 1995; 376: 572-575
        • Durstewitz D.
        • Seamans J.K.
        • Sejnowski T.J.
        Dopamine-mediated stabilization of delay-period activity in a network model of prefrontal cortex.
        J Neurophysiol. 2000; 83: 1733-1750
        • Arnsten A.F.
        • Cai J.X.
        • Murphy B.L.
        • Goldman-Rakic P.S.
        Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys.
        Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1994; 116: 143-151
        • Abi-Dargham A.
        • Mawlawi O.
        • Lombardo I.
        • Gil R.
        • Martinez D.
        • Huang Y.
        • et al.
        Prefrontal dopamine D1 receptors and working memory in schizophrenia.
        J Neurosci. 2002; 22: 3708-3719
        • Yang S.N.
        Sustained enhancement of AMPA receptor- and NMDA receptor-mediated currents induced by dopamine D1/D5 receptor activation in the hippocampus: An essential role of postsynaptic Ca2+.
        Nature. 2000; 376: 572-575
        • Cepeda C.
        • Levine M.S.
        Where do you think you are going?.
        Sci STKE. 2006; 2006: pe20
        • Tseng K.Y.
        • O'Donnell P.
        Dopamine-glutamate interactions controlling prefrontal cortical pyramidal cell excitability involve multiple signaling mechanisms.
        J Neurosci. 2004; 24: 5131-5139