After more than 4 decades, most psychiatrists recognize biological psychiatry as a
major component of training, practice, and research in their field. By contrast, most
biologists, at least behavioral biologists, would view some of the basic tenets of
this field as completely alien or antiquated. Nowhere is this gap between biology
and psychiatry more evident than in the concept of “animal models.” This editorial
makes two points. First, it argues that biological psychiatry can learn much from
modern comparative neurobiology, which studies the neural basis of species-typical
behaviors rather than looking for phenocopies of human behavior. Second, it argues
that traditional animal models might be mechanistically misleading, but the use of
model organisms to understand the pathophysiology of mental disorders will be critical
as clinical studies identify genetic alleles and cellular changes that confer risk
for mental disorders.
To read this article in full you will need to make a payment
Purchase one-time access:
Academic & Personal: 24 hour online accessCorporate R&D Professionals: 24 hour online accessOne-time access price info
- For academic or personal research use, select 'Academic and Personal'
- For corporate R&D use, select 'Corporate R&D Professionals'
Subscribe:
Subscribe to Biological PsychiatryAlready a print subscriber? Claim online access
Already an online subscriber? Sign in
Register: Create an account
Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect
References
- On the Origin Of Species.John Murray, London1859
- Neural circuits and behaviour: Developmental and evolutionary perspectives.Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005; 15: 683-685
- The neurobiology of attachment.Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001; 2: 129-136
- Natural variation in a neuropeptide Y receptor homolog modifies socal behavior and food response in C. elegans.Cell. 1998; 94: 679-689
- Evolution at two levels: On genes and form.PLoS Biology. 2005; 3: e245
- Taking the measure of diversity: Comparative alternatives to the model-animal paradigm in cortical neuroscience.Brain Behav Evol. 2000; 55: 287-299
- Why seeing is believing: Merging auditory and visual worlds.Neuron. 2005; 48: 489-496
- Patterning the brain of the zebrafish embryo.Annu Rev Neurosci. 1993; 16: 707-732
- Nature’s gift to science (Nobel lecture).Chembiochem. 2003; 4: 683-687
- Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: On the matter of their convergence.Mol Psychiatry. 2005; 10: 40-68
- Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders.Nature. 1997; 386: 824-827
- Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression.Neuron. 2005; 45: 651-660
- Comprehensive analysis of combinatorial regulation using the transcriptional regulatory network of yeast.J Mol Biol. 2006; 360: 213-227
- Ancient noncoding elements conserved in the human genome.Science. 2006; 314: 1892
- Localization of a stable neural correlate of associative memory.Science. 2007; 317: 1230-1233
- On the nature of the BOLD fMRI contrast mechanism.Magn Reson Imaging. 2004; 22: 1517-1531
- Backwardness of human neuroanatomy.Nature. 1992; 361: 109-110
- Come FLY with us: Toward understanding fragile X syndrome.Genes Brain Behav. 2005; 4: 385-392
- Mouse models of neurofibromatosis type I: Bridging the GAP.Trends Mol Med. 2003; 9: 19-23
- Reversal of neurological defects in a mouse model of rett syndrome.Science. 2007; 315: 1143-1147
- The mGlur theory of fragile X mental retardation.Trends Neuro. 2004; 27: 370-377
- A potential animal model for Lesch-Nyhan syndrome through introduction of HPRT mutations into mice.Nature. 1987; 326: 295-298
Article info
Identification
Copyright
© 2007 Society of Biological Psychiatry. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.