Abstract
Background: Unpredictability has been postulated to be fundamental to anxiety and
mood disorders. The origin of this unpredictability remains obscure. Because classical
conditioning promotes predictability, this study investigated whether failure to learn
conditioned stimulus (CS)-unconditioned stimulus (US) relationship during fear conditioning
increased anxiety and avoidance.
Methods: Healthy subjects participated in two similar differential fear conditioning
sessions separated by 1 week (n = 72) or a month (n = 61) in which one of two conditioned stimuli was associated with a shock/US. Following
initial acquisition, subjects’ awareness of CS-US relationship was assessed. Conditioned
responses (CR) to the CS and to the experimental context were examined using the startle
reflex and the skin conductance. Avoidance was operationally defined as failure to
return for the second session.
Results: Only aware subjects showed differential CR. In the unaware subjects, the
deficit in differential conditioning was associated with increased signs of anxiety
during the first and second sessions. In addition, there was greater avoidance in
the unaware subjects.
Conclusions: Deficits in explicit cue fear conditioning can enhance anxiety. These
findings are consistent with theories that associate anxiety and mood disorders with
perceived unpredictability. Contextual conditioning models may be relevant to study
chronic forms of anxiety.
Keywords
To read this article in full you will need to make a payment
Purchase one-time access:
Academic & Personal: 24 hour online accessCorporate R&D Professionals: 24 hour online accessOne-time access price info
- For academic or personal research use, select 'Academic and Personal'
- For corporate R&D use, select 'Corporate R&D Professionals'
Subscribe:
Subscribe to Biological PsychiatryAlready a print subscriber? Claim online access
Already an online subscriber? Sign in
Register: Create an account
Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect
References
- Contextual fear-potentiated startle conditioning in humans.Psychophysiology. 2001; 38: 383-390
- Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC1994
- Vigilant and nonvigilant coping strategies and psychophysiological stress reactions during the anticipation of electric shock.J Pers Soc Psychol. 1972; 23: 128-141
- Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory.Am Psychol. 2000; 55: 1247-1263
- Anxiety Disorders and Phobias. Bantam Books, New York1985
- Long-term retention of fear-potentiated startle following a short training session.Anim Learn Behav. 1990; 18: 462-468
- Expectancy bias in trait anxiety.J Abnorm Psychol. 1996; 105: 637-647
- The extended amygdala.Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999; 877: 281-291
- Construct validity of recall and recognition postconditioning measures of awareness.J Exp Psychol. 1973; 98: 308-315
- The conditioning model of neurosis.Behav Brain Sci. 1979; 2: 155-199
- Inhibition, symptoms, and anxiety (1925–1926). Vol 20. Hogart Press, London1959
- Guidelines for human electromyographic research.Psychophysiology. 1986; 23: 567-589
- Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle and skin conductance in humans.Psychophysiology. 2001; 38: 807-815
- Fear-potentiated startle conditioning in humans.Psychophysiology. 1997; 34: 451-458
- Experimental studies in affective processes.J Exp Psychol. 1943; 33: 257-284
- Affective learning.Psychophysiology. 1996; 33: 698-710
- From metapsychology to molecular biology.Am J Psychiatry. 1983; 140: 1277-1293
- Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear.Science. 1992; 256: 675-677
- Conditioning and generalization of unconscious anxiety.Science. 1954; 120: 1045-1052
- Role of the hippocampus, the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and the amygdala in the excitatory effect of corticotropin-releasing hormone on the acoustic startle reflex.J Neurosci. 1997; 17: 6434-6446
- Stressor controllability, cognition and fear.in: Madden J.I Neurobiology of Learning, Emotion and Affect. Raven Press, New York1991: 159-193
- Learning and unlearning fear.Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1990; 14: 365-384
- Vulnerability factors among children at risk for anxiety disorders.Biol Psychiatry. 1999; 46: 1523-1535
- Unpredictable and uncontrollable events.J Abnorm Psychol. 1978; 87: 256-271
- The role of cognitive biases in the origins and maintenance of fear and anxiety disorders.in: Archer T Nilsson L.G Aversion, Avoidance, and Anxiety. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ1989: 195-221
- Conditioning and ethological models of anxiety disorders.in: Hope D.A Perspective on Anxiety, Panic, and Fear. Vol 43. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln1996: 135-210
- A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent.Psychol Rev. 1939; 46: 553-565
- Background stimuli and the inter-stimulus interval during pavlovian conditioning.Q J Exper Psychol. 1975; 27: 387-392
- Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning.Behav Neurosci. 1992; 106: 274-285
- Three components of the classically conditioned GSR in human subjects.J Exp Psychol. 1967; 73: 247-256
- A theory of Pavlovian conditioning.in: Black A.H Prokasy W.F Classical Conditioning II Current Theory and Research. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York1972: 64-99
- Memory retrieval deficits based upon altered contextual cues.Psychol Bull. 1984; 96: 152-165
- The safety signal hypothesis.in: Davis H Hurwitz H.M.B Operant-Pavlovian Interactions. Hillsdale, New York1977: 165-187
- Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologist Press, Palo Alto, CA1983
Article info
Publication history
Accepted:
December 6,
2001
Received in revised form:
November 30,
2001
Received:
September 17,
2001
Identification
Copyright
© 2002 Society of Biological Psychiatry. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.